Introduction to the Italian Translation of Darwin s Cathedral

Similar documents
HUMAN NATURE REVIEW ISSN Book Review

Religious Belief and Atheism are not Mutually Exclusive. Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford and Todd K. Shackelford. Oakland University

Intelligent Design. Kevin delaplante Dept. of Philosophy & Religious Studies

THE IMPACT OF DARWIN S THEORIES. Darwin s Theories and Human Nature

The Biological Foundation of Bioethics

Are There Philosophical Conflicts Between Science & Religion? (Participant's Guide)

Prentice Hall Biology 2004 (Miller/Levine) Correlated to: Idaho Department of Education, Course of Study, Biology (Grades 9-12)

Can You Believe in God and Evolution?

382 Human Nature / Winter Testing Major Evolutionary Hypotheses about Religion with a Random Sample. David Sloan Wilson Binghamton University

Can You Believe In God and Evolution?

Darwin s Theologically Unsettling Ideas. John F. Haught Georgetown University

Hindu Paradigm of Evolution

Hume's Is/Ought Problem. Ruse and Wilson. Moral Philosophy as Applied Science. Naturalistic Fallacy

FAITH & reason. The Pope and Evolution Anthony Andres. Winter 2001 Vol. XXVI, No. 4

SOCIOLOGICAL ANALYSIS Michaelmas 2017 Dr Michael Biggs. 7. Evolution. SociologicalAnalysis.shtml!

Hume s Is/Ought Problem. Ruse and Wilson. Moral Philosophy as Applied Science. Naturalistic Fallacy

Vol. 29 No. 22 Cover date: 15 November 2007

Religious and non religious beliefs and teachings about the origin of the universe.

Charles Robert Darwin ( ) Born in Shrewsbury, England. His mother died when he was eight, a

INTRODUCTION to ICONS of EVOLUTION: Science or Myth? Why much of what we teach about evolution is wrong

Atheism, Ideology and Belief: What Do We Believe in When We Don t Believe in God? Dr Michael S Burdett University of Oxford University of St Andrews

Plantinga, Van Till, and McMullin. 1. What is the conflict Plantinga proposes to address in this essay? ( )

Perspectives on Imitation

IS THE SCIENTIFIC METHOD A MYTH? PERSPECTIVES FROM THE HISTORY AND PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE

ONTOLOGICAL PROBLEMS OF PLURALIST RESEARCH METHODOLOGIES

World-Wide Ethics. Chapter One. Individual Subjectivism

SOCI : SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION TR 9:30 10:50 ENV 125 Fall, 2013

I-Search: Are Religion and Science Compatible? with them. This would all change with the pursuit of a higher education.

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION (sample lower level undergraduate course)

Marcel Sarot Utrecht University Utrecht, The Netherlands NL-3508 TC. Introduction

STH TT824/TT954: Scientific Approaches to Religion

Media Critique #5. Exercise #8 4/29/2010. Critique the Bullshit!

Darwinist Arguments Against Intelligent Design Illogical and Misleading

It Depends on What You Mean by Altruism

BIO 221 Invertebrate Zoology I Spring Course Information. Course Website. Lecture 1. Stephen M. Shuster Professor of Invertebrate Zoology

Human Nature & Human Diversity: Sex, Love & Parenting; Morality, Religion & Race. Course Description

Is Evolution Incompatible with Intelligent Design? Outline

Scientific Dimensions of the Debate. 1. Natural and Artificial Selection: the Analogy (17-20)

A Biblical Perspective on the Philosophy of Science

The Debate Between Evolution and Intelligent Design Rick Garlikov

Information and the Origin of Life

Outline Lesson 5 -Science: What is True? A. Psalm 19:1-4- "The heavens declare the Glory of God" -General Revelation

Lecture 5.2Dawkins and Dobzhansky. Richard Dawkin s explanation of Cumulative Selection, in The Blind Watchmaker video.

Wow, this is it.. Turn in your Divination project. Today; lecture then our last group project. Then final prep

Roots of Dialectical Materialism*

Strange bedfellows or Siamese twins? The search for the sacred in practical theology and psychology of religion

Why God Is Watching Supernatural Punishment and the Evolution of Cooperation. Dominic D. P. Johnson University of Oxford

Introduction to Evolution. DANILO V. ROGAYAN JR. Faculty, Department of Natural Sciences

Reviewed by Eva Kundtová Klocová, LEVYNA Laboratory for the Experimental Research of Religion, Masaryk University,

From: Michael Huemer, Ethical Intuitionism (2005)

Review of Erik J. Wielenberg: Robust Ethics: The Metaphysics and Epistemology of Godless Normative Realism

CURE 1111 The Study of Religion Second Term

EXERCISES, QUESTIONS, AND ACTIVITIES My Answers

Science and the Christian Faith. Brent Royuk June 11, 2006

Section 1 of chapter 1 of The Moral Sense advances the thesis that we have a

prominent atheists/evolutionists have released books that have rermained on the New York

Ground Work 01 part one God His Existence Genesis 1:1/Psalm 19:1-4

God After Darwin. 1. Evolution s s Challenge to Faith. July 23, to 9:50 am in the Parlor All are welcome!

Science and Christianity. Do you have to choose? In my opinion no

Post-Modernism and Science: Challenges to 21 st Century Christian Witness

Scientific Method and Research Ethics

The Science of Creation and the Flood. Introduction to Lesson 7

From Planting to Movements: The Role of City Networks

The tribulations of Rationality in Philosophy, Economics and Biology by Alex Kacelnik University of Oxford

EVOLUTION FOR EVERYONE: AN UNDERGRADUATE PERSPECTIVE

Virtue Ethics without Character Traits

Naturalism Primer. (often equated with materialism )

In Gods We Trust: The Evolutionary Landscape Of Religion (Evolution And Cognition) By Scott Atran READ ONLINE

What Is Science? Mel Conway, Ph.D.

Common Morality Approaches for Ethics of Environmental Health

American Scientist. A reprint from. the magazine of Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society

Time is limited. Define your terms. Give short and conventional definitions. Use reputable sources.

Coptic Orthodox Diocese of the Southern United States Evangelism & Apologetics Conference. Copyright by George Bassilios, 2014

Discussion Questions Confident Faith, Mark Mittelberg. Chapter 9 Assessing the Six Faith Paths

Are Humans Always Selfish? OR Is Altruism Possible?

Module 4: Argument. In ecology and biology, arguments are often used to:

FLAME TEEN HANDOUT Week 18 Religion and Science

An Inquiry into the Diverse Articulations of Science & Religion in Contemporary Life

Integrated Studies 002: Human Morality and Emotions University of Pennsylvania Spring 2017

Mètode Science Studies Journal ISSN: Universitat de València España

Ursuline College Accelerated Program

Chapter 2 Ethical Concepts and Ethical Theories: Establishing and Justifying a Moral System

1/18/2009. Signatories include:

Dawkins has claimed that evolution has been observed. If it s true, doesn t this mean that creationism has been disproved?

The Evolution of Religion

UC Davis Department of Religious Studies. Undergraduate Writing Prize. Emergence, Persistence and the Secular Utility of Religion

Moral Objectivism. RUSSELL CORNETT University of Calgary

What About Evolution?

Understanding Truth Scott Soames Précis Philosophy and Phenomenological Research Volume LXV, No. 2, 2002

Has not Science Debunked Biblical Christianity?

Explore the Christian rationale for environmental ethics and assess its strengths and weaknesses.

Nancey Murphy, Bodies and Souls, or Spirited Bodies? (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2006). Pp. x Hbk, Pbk.

Brad Weslake, Department of Philosophy. Darwin Day, 12 February 2012

Christopher Heard Pepperdine University Malibu, California

Review of Who Rules in Science?, by James Robert Brown

SEMESTER AT SEA COURSE SYLLABUS University of Virginia, Academic Sponsor

Glossary. Arabah: The hot and dry elongated depression through which the Jordan River flows from the Sea of Galilee to the Dead Sea.

There is a God. A Much-Maligned Convert

ASA 2017 Annual Meeting. Stephen Dilley, Ph.D., and Nicholas Tafacory St Edward s University

The Answer from Science

Transcription:

Introduction to the Italian Translation of Darwin s Cathedral I thank Gilberto Corbellini for the opportunity to provide an update on Darwin s Cathedral on the occasion of its Italian translation. It was doubly controversial upon its publication in 2002. First, studying religion from an evolutionary perspective was regarded as a fringe topic. Second, group selection has been controversial within evolutionary theory for many decades. Explaining religion as (primarily) a group-level adaptation therefore made me an outlaw among outlaws. Today, I am pleased to report that Darwin s Cathedral has become less controversial in both senses. Studying religion from an evolutionary perspective has become fully respectable; indeed, I am confident that it will become the theoretical framework of choice, as for the study of all other aspects of humanity. And group selection has finally become respectable again, as Edward O. Wilson and I review in two recent articles titled Rethinking the Theoretical Foundation of Sociobiology (2007) and Evolution For the Good of the Group (2008). Why is evolutionary theory so insightful for the study of religion? It provides a number of major hypotheses for the evolution of all traits. Is a given trait an adaptation that evolved by contributing to survival and reproduction? If so, what is the unit of selection? Did it evolve by virtue of causing groups to survive and reproduce better than other groups (a product of between-group selection)? Or by causing individuals to survive and reproduce better than other individuals within the same group (a product of within-group selection)? With cultural evolution there is a third possibility; because cultural traits are transmitted from person to person, they bear an intriguing resemblance to parasitic organisms. Just as parasites often evolve to benefit themselves at the expense of their hosts, cultural traits can potentially evolve merely to perpetuate themselves, at the expense of both human individuals and groups. In addition to these three adaptation hypotheses, there are also a number of nonadaptation hypotheses. Not everything that evolves is adaptive, as the late evolutionist Stephen Jay Gould never tired of pointing out. A given trait might have been adaptive in the past but not the present, such as our eating habits, which make great sense in environments of food shortage but are killing us in today s fast food environment. Or, a trait can be a costly byproduct of another trait. Moths, for example, navigate by celestial light sources such as the moon and stars, which is highly adaptive, but also causes them to become fatally attracted to earthly light sources such as a street lamp or a candle flame. Finally, some traits are neutral with respect to survival and reproduction and merely drift into a population. Many genes are selectively neutral, and perhaps at least some culturally evolved traits associated with religion are neutral as well. Another major insight provided by evolutionary theory is the distinction between proximate and ultimate causation. Proximate causation addresses the question of how a given trait exists in physical terms, which involves a consideration of genetics, development, physiology, neurobiology, and so on. Ultimate causation addresses the question of why a given trait exists, compared to many other traits that could have existed. This involves a consideration of how the trait contributed to survival and

reproduction in the past (adaptation) random factors (drift) and historical factors (phylogeny). Everything that evolves requires both explanations, which do not substitute for each other and must be studied in a complementary fashion. These major hypotheses and the proximate/ultimate distinction organize the study of the biological sciences. Remarkably, they serve just as well to organize the study of humanity, including the study of religion. They can even classify past theories of religion that were formulated without using the E-word, as I describe for the tradition of functionalism represented by Emile Durkheim and more modern sociological theories of religion represented by Rodney Stark in chapter 2 of Darwin s Cathedral. Thus, one contribution of evolutionary theory for the study of religion as a natural phenomenon is to provide an elegant accounting system that includes all reasonable possibilities. The next challenge is to determine which of the major hypotheses are most salient for the genetically and culturally evolved traits associated with religion. Evolution is a messy and multifactorial process. Religion is a notoriously fuzzy set of traits. All of the major hypotheses are therefore likely to be relevant to some degree, but some will be more relevant than others when the elements of religion are examined on a case-by-case basis. Until recently, evolutionists who study religion could be described as agreeing about the accounting system but disagreeing about which major hypotheses are most relevant. According to authors such as Pascal Boyer (2001) and Scott Atran (2002) religion is primarily a byproduct of psychological adaptations that evolved in non-religious contexts. According to authors such as Richard Dawkins (2006) and Daniel Dennett (2006), religion is primarily a cultural parasite with negative effects on human welfare. Other authors interpret empirical evidence for benefits of religion, such as health benefits, as evidence for individual-level selection. All of these seem to stand in contrast with my own emphasis on religion as adaptive at the group level. This lack of consensus might seem like a weakness and would be if it continued indefinitely but it can also be regarded as the scientific process in action. The best that a theory can do is outline a number of plausible alternative hypotheses; then empirical research must take over to evaluate the alternatives and formulate a more refined set of hypotheses. Progress results with each cycle of hypothesis formation and testing. I m glad to report that this kind of progress has been taking place within the nascent field of evolutionary religious studies. A consensus is forming on the following major issues, as I report in my recent article titled Evolution and Religion: The Transformation of the Obvious (Wilson 2008), which can be briefly summarized here. The communitarian nature of most enduring religions can be accepted at face value. Durkheim was right that religions are (largely) built and designed to provide secular utility to their members. In my survey of a sample of religions drawn randomly from Eliade s (1987 ) Encyclopedia of Religion, which I began in Darwin s Cathedral and continued in Wilson (2005), the communitarian nature of most religions in the sample was undeniable, including religions such as Jainism that superficially appear to support the cultural parasite hypothesis. This does not mean that we can return to the tradition of functionalism in its original form, but we do need to create a tradition of neo-

functionalism based upon the concept of major transitions, one of the most important developments in the history of evolutionary theory. Religions reflect multi-level selection, not just group selection. A major transition occurs when selection within groups is suppressed, enabling selection among groups to become the dominant evolutionary force. Selection within-groups is never entirely eliminated, however, even in multi-cellular biological organisms. Thus, no one should be surprised to see free-riding, cheating, and active exploitation in religious groups, just like all other human social organizations. Moreover, religions often represent a multi-tiered hierarchy of groups within groups within groups. Cooperation and social control come naturally at the level of small-scale groups but become increasingly difficult at larger scales. The history of the rise and fall of religions, along with other human social organizations, can be regarded as an exquisitely detailed fossil record of multilevel cultural evolution (Turchin 2005). Between-group selection sometimes--but not always--takes the form of betweengroup conflict. Popular discourse on religion invariably includes the observation that religious groups often engage in between-group conflict, as if this is somehow hypocritical or antithetical to the true nature of religion. This evaluation is supremely naïve from a multilevel evolutionary perspective. The general rule multilevel selection is that adaptation at a given level requires a process of selection at the same level and tends to be undermined by selection at lower levels. This means that the morality and prosociality encouraged by religions are primarily within the group and adapted for between-group competition in the broad sense. However, the broad sense does not necessarily mean violent between-group conflict. Darwin was careful to stress that nature is not always red in tooth can claw. A drought-resistant plant can outcompete a drought-susceptible plant in a desert environment without any direct interactions among the plants. Similarly, groups can outcompete other groups simply by functioning better as collective units. This is a form of between-group competition that we should all welcome who wants to be a member of a dysfunctional group? Relationships among biological species span the full range from predation, to parasitism, to competition, to coexisting without directly interacting, to facilitation, to obligate mutualism. Relations among human groups, including religious groups, can be expected to span the same range. In my random sample of religions, I was gratified to discover that the majority did not spread by violent between-group conflict think of early Christianity and Mormonism. Individual thriving does not equal individual-level selection. Suppose that I give you a million dollars, which you invest wisely in your own welfare. You thrive as an individual, but that does not provide evidence that you have a trait enabling you to survive and reproduce better than other members of your group. On the contrary, we need to explain how giving people like me persist in the world. By the same token, religions often provide services that enable their members to thrive as individuals in purely secular terms. The mere fact of individual thriving does not provide any evidence whatsoever that the elements of religion responsible for the thriving evolved by a process of withingroup selection. On the contrary, they are much more likely to result from public goods that require time, energy, and risk on the part of someone to produce, and which persist primarily on the strength of between-group selection.

Adaptation and byproduct hypotheses need to be evaluated separately for genetic and cultural evolution. According to authors such as Boyer (2001) and Atran (2002), some elements of religion evolved by genetic evolution for reasons that have nothing to do with religion per se, such as a mental bias toward attributing agency to events. Their religious expression therefore qualifies as a byproduct as far as genetic evolution is concerned. However, genetic byproducts can easily become adaptations as far as cultural evolution is concerned. Most adaptations, for biological and cultural evolution alike, are derived from structures and behaviors that served different purposes (or no purpose) in the past. The status of a given element of religion as an adaptation vs. byproduct therefore must be determined separately for biological and cultural evolution. This means that the genetic byproduct hypothesis associated with authors such as Boyer and Atran can be compatible with the cultural group-level adaptation hypothesis that I develop in Darwin s Cathedral. There is very little evidence for the cultural parasite hypothesis. The titles chosen by Richard Dawkins (The God Delusion), and Daniel Dennett (Breaking the Spell) for their books illustrate their reliance on the cultural parasite hypothesis as their primary explanation of religion. This is a perfectly respectable hypothesis, as I stressed earlier. However, there is very little empirical evidence that religious beliefs harm the believer in the same way that disease organisms harm their hosts. Examples that superficially give this appearance, such as suicide terrorism and the extreme acetic practices associated with religions such as Jainism, turn out to be much more complicated and oriented toward Durkheim s concept of secular utility upon closer examination. We should look for examples of religious drift. No one would claim that religion in its entirety is selectively neutral, but the concept of religious drift might still explain particular elements of religion. I recently encountered a potential example at a workshop on the Golden Rule in religious traditions around the world (Neusner and Chilton 2009 ). It is often remarked that all major religions embody the injunction to Do Unto Others, but the workshop revealed a more interesting and complicated story. Most enduring religions are indeed adaptive at the group level, but this requires a large number of rules that are employed in a context-specific manner, not any single rule, as I stressed in my contribution titled The Golden Rules of Religion (Wilson 2009). Moreover, as one of many rules, the specific injunction to treat others as one would like oneself treated is featured much more prominently in some religions than others. Judaism is centered on the principle of measure for measure which is not the same as the Golden Rule. The Golden Rule would have been highly maladaptive in Hellenic society. Confusianism prescribes rights and responsibilities to people occupying different positions in a stratified society, and so on. Thus, the Golden Rule is hugely consequential and therefore has a distribution and abundance, (to use an ecological term) in how it is invoked contextually, both within and between religions. A more nuanced distinction, however, is between the positive and negative version of the Golden Rule. According to the workshop participants, most religious traditions do not make an important distinction between the two versions, which might therefore count as an example of religious drift. It will be interesting to test this hypothesis in detail using the same methodological tools that evolutionary biologists have developed to study genetic drift.

For more on these and other topics, please visit the website that I and my colleagues have created to establish the new field of Evolutionary Religious studies, including a model research project on religious conceptions of the afterlife from an evolutionary perspective (http://evolution.binghamton.edu/religion). For a thorough introduction and examination of the foundational issues, however, Darwin s Cathedral is still the best way to start. I am delighted that it is now available to an Italian audience. David Sloan Wilson Binghamton, NY December 26, 2008 Literature Cited Atran, S. (2002). In Gods we Trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion Explained. New York: Basic Books. Dawkins, R. (2006). The God Delusion. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Dennett, D. C. (2006). Breaking the spell: religion as a natural phenomenon. New York: Viking. Eliade, M. (Ed.). (1987). The Encylopedia of Religion. New York: MacMillan. Neusner, J., & Chilton, B. D. (Eds.). (2009). The Golden Rule: The Ethics of Reciprocity in World Religions. New York: Continuum. Turchin, P. (2005). War and Peace and War: Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pi Press. Wilson, D. S. (2005). Testing major evolutionary hypotheses about religion with a random sample. Human Nature, 16, 382-409. Wilson, D. S. (2008). Evolution and Religion: The Transformation of the obvious. In J. Bulbulia, R. Sosis, E. Harris, R. Genet, C. Genet & K. Wyman (Eds.), The Evolution of Religion: Studies, Theories, Critiques (pp. 11-18). Santa Margarita, CA: Collins Foundation Press. Wilson, D. S. (2009). The Golden Rules of Religion. In J. Neusner & B. D. Chilton (Eds.), The Golden Rule: The Ethics of Reciprocity in World Religions. New York: Continuum. Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82, 327-348. Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2008). Evolution "For the Good of the Group". American Scientist, 96, 380-389.