LINGUIST 601 Assigned on November 30, 2006 Due on December 12, Assignment 11

Similar documents
May 10, 2007 McGill University. Little or Nothing. Rajesh Bhatt. University of Massachusetts at Amherst

RECIPIENT ENCODING IN SOUTHERN SELKUP ANJA HARDER, UNIVERSITY OF HAMBURG

Introduction to Transformational Grammar, LINGUIST 601 December 3, Wh-Movement

Factivity and Presuppositions David Schueler University of Minnesota, Twin Cities LSA Annual Meeting 2013

PASSIVES IN SOME SOUTH ASIAN LANGUAGES: A COMPARATIVE INVESTIGATION

Infinitives, gerunds, participles

CAS LX 522 Syntax I Fall 2000 November 6, 2000 Paul Hagstrom Week 9: Binding Theory. (8) John likes him.

Anaphora Resolution in Hindi Language

The role of animacy and definiteness in the clitic-dp nexus

Unit Outline Time Content Classical Strategies/ Instruction

JOURNAL OF LINGUISTICS

Four Proposals for German Clause Structure

TURCOLOGICA. Herausgegeben von Lars Johanson. Band 98. Harrassowitz Verlag Wiesbaden

Reference Resolution. Regina Barzilay. February 23, 2004

Reference Resolution. Announcements. Last Time. 3/3 first part of the projects Example topics

Pronominal, temporal and descriptive anaphora

ACD in AP? Richard K. Larson. Stony Brook University

Introduction to Koiné Greek

What is infinitival to?

MODAL VERBS. The modal verbs are a special group of auxiliary verbs. They are different from most other verbs in four ways: + infinitives without to

Extra Syntax Exercises 5

Caught in the Act. The Evolution of the Object and Pronominal Copula Clitics in the Neo-Aramaic Dialects. SLE 2015, 4 September, Leiden University

Exhaustification over Questions in Japanese

Latin Alive! Book 2 Yearlong

Subject Index. Index

B. Diagram the following verses from 1 Cor 1:10-31, marking sentence connectors with a D. AEYEl.,

Holger Diessel University of Jena.

Reconsidering Raising and Experiencers in English

Final Exam due on December 13, 2001

"The functions of short-distance demonstratives in the pronominal and the verbal system of Xinaliq"

GRAMMAR IV HIGH INTERMEDIATE

PASSIVE REPORT STRUCTURE / IMPERSONAL PASSIVE

Dative External Possessor Constructions in Sidaama

Note: NEW = teachers should expect the grammar point to be new to most students at that level who have followed the ELI curriculum.

If I hadn t studied as much as I did, I wouldn t have passed my exams.

Reminder: Yes-no questions

BOOK 1 OF PLATO S REPUBLIC: A WORD BY WORD GUIDE TO TRANSLATION (VOL 2: CHAPTERS 13 24) BrownWalker.com

Syntactic Conditions on Null Arguments in Indo-European Bible Translations

Properties as anaphors

Helpful Hints for doing Philosophy Papers (Spring 2000)

Unit 6 Transformation of Sentences

Front Range Bible Institute

9 th Grade English Placement Test

Language Diversity friend or foe? Michael Cysouw Philipps-Universität Marburg

Category Mistakes in M&E

Jenney s First Year Latin Lesson 40

Faculty of Oriental Studies. Setting conventions for the MSt in Jewish Studies,

Long-distance anaphora: comparing Mandarin Chinese with Iron Range English 1

Book Reviews. The Lost Sermons of C. H. Spurgeon, Volume 1. Nashville: B&H, Edited by Christian George. 400 pages. $59.99

The Development of Binding Theory Handout #1

Index. B Backgrounding, 305, 311 Bohairic, 208 Burgundian, 208, 209 Burgundy, 12

BBC LEARNING ENGLISH The Grammar Gameshow

Binding of Indeterminate Pronouns and Clause Structure in Japanese by Hideki Kishimoto, in press, LI

Is Sunday Called the Sabbath in the New Testament?

Jehovah s Witnesses and John 1:1. The un-edited excerpts from the Jehovah s Witnesses pamphlet Should You Believe the Trinity? are in red.

By the Time Viewing relative progress or completion

A Typology of Clause Combining

SPIRITUAL GIFTS. Pastor William E. Wenstrom Jr. WENSTROM BIBLE MINISTRIES Marion, Iowa 2016 William E. Wenstrom, Jr.

PRESENT SIMPLE PRESENT CONTINUOUS PAST SIMPLE PRESENT PERFECT SIMPLE. ANGLEŠČINA slovnica. The Present Simple is used to talk about:

John chapter 15 has been a much-debated text. The controversy largely centers on the first six to

Wayne L. Atchison October 17, 2007

Ling 98a: The Meaning of Negation (Week 1)

PASSIVE REPORT STRUCTURE / IMPERSONAL PASSIVE

Latin I Unit Plan and Curriculum Map Course Overview:

APPLICATIVES IN NON-CANONICAL SUBJECT CONSTRUCTIONS

Romans Chapter Translation

Diving In: Getting the Most from God s Word Investigate the Word (Observation and Study) Teaching: Paul Lamey

Anaphoric Deflationism: Truth and Reference

Comments on Lasersohn

Qal Imperative, Qal Jussive, Qal Cohortative, Negative Commands, Volitive Sequences Mark Francois. Hebrew Grammar

On the syntax of yes and no in English

I. PATTERNS OF CONNECTION

Logophors, variable binding and the interpretation of have. *

CAS LX 523 Syntax II February 10, 2009 Prep for week 5: The fine structure of the left periphery

Replies to Giuliano Torrengo, Dan Zeman and Vasilis Tsompanidis

(5) Noi îţi părem (ţie) să lucrăm bine.

AUXILIARIES AND MODALS

A Short Addition to Length: Some Relative Frequencies of Circumstantial Structures

Based on the translation by E. M. Edghill, with minor emendations by Daniel Kolak.

NULL CASE, PRO, and the SECOND DATIVE (mostly Russian)

Chapter 46. Adjectives - part 2 Π", ΠΠ* ~" "

GENERAL EDUCATION AND TRAINING

Russell: On Denoting

LGCS 199DR: Independent Study in Pragmatics

Commentary From the Margins

Chapter 2 Truth Predicates in Natural Language

Russell on Plurality

Outline of today s lecture

Prentice Hall U.S. History Modern America 2013

Scope and Sequence 1

Dative Subjects: Historical Change Visualized

Core and peripheral da-ves: da-ve agreement drop in Basque

CONDITIONAL SENTENCES CONDITIONAL SENTENCES

1. Introduction Formal deductive logic Overview

Zero Conditionals. Check point Circle T (True) or F (False). T F The man may not be able to board the plane.

Presupposition and Rules for Anaphora

Quantifiers: Their Semantic Type (Part 3) Heim and Kratzer Chapter 6

Houghton Mifflin English 2004 Houghton Mifflin Company Grade Six. correlated to. TerraNova, Second Edition Level 16

QUESTION TAGS

Questions. Yes-no: Chamorro kao left-peripherally (cf. Irish a N ) (1) Kao un-kuentusi gui? C35 Q agr-speak.to him/her 'Did you speak to him/her?

Transcription:

LINGUIST 601 Assigned on November 30, 2006 Due on December 12, 2006 Assignment 11 1 Icelandic Quirky Case and Control Icelandic presents a rich system of non-nominative subjects which can have a variety of oblique cases, sometimes called Quirky Cases. (1) a. Hana/*H ún vantadi vinnu. she.acc/she.no lacked job She lacked a job. b. Henni/*Hún leiddist. she.dat/she.nom bored She was bored. c. Hennar/*Hún var getid. she.gen/she.nom was mentioned She was mentioned (by someone). The availability of quirky case is predicate dependent. It depends upon the predicate at hand whether the subject will bear a quirky case and what particular quirky case it will bear. What are the implications of the following facts for the theory of PRO? How does PRO in Icelandic differ from PRO in English? When do we get PRO and when do we get overt subjects in Icelandic? Provide an analysis of the Icelandic facts. For concreteness, you can assume that information about Quirky Case is available at the point where the subject is merged. 1.1 Controlling Quirky Subjects Positions that receive Quirky Case cannot be overtly realized in the infinitival clauses in (2). We could say that these positions are occupied by PRO. (2) a. Hún vonast til [ad she.nom hopes for to She hopes not to lack a job. b. Hana langar ekki til [ad she.acc wants not for to PRO vanta lack She does not want to be bored. c. PaD it vaeri gaman [ad were nice to PRO leidast]. bore ekki vinnu]. not job PRO verda getid]. be mentioned It would be nice to be mentioned. (the exact surface position of PRO is open to debate. For all we know it precedes ad.)

1.2 Floating Quantifiers and Quirky Subjects Floating Quantifiers in Icelandic inflect for case, number, and gender. They show agreement with the NP they are associated with. (3) a. Strákarnir komast allir í skóla. the-boys.nom got all.nom.mpl to school The boys all managed to get to school. b. Strákana vantadi alla í skólann. the-boys.acc lacked all.acc.mpl in the-school The boys were all absent from the school. c. Strákunum leidist öllum í skóla. the-boys.dat bored all.dat.mpl in school The boys were all bored in school. d. Strákanna var allra getid í raedunni. the-boys.gen were all.gen.ml mentioned in the-speech The boys were all mentioned in the speech. Floating quantifiers are also possible in control constructions where they display the following behaviour: (4) a. Strákarnir vonast til [ad The boys hope to all get to school. Strákarnir vonast til [ad PRO komast allir get PRO vanta lack The boys hope to not be all absent from school. Strákarnir vonast til [ad PRO leidast bore The boys hope to not be all bored in school. Strákarnir vonast til [ad PRO verda allra ve The boys hope to be all mentioned in the speech. í skóla]. all.nom.mpl to school ekki alla í skólann. not all.acc.pl to the-school ekki öllum í skóla]. not all.dat.mpl in school getid í raedunni. all.gen.pl mentioned in the-speech 1.3 Predicate Agreement and Quirky Subjects You need to formulate an account of agreement in Icelandic that extends to both finite and nonfinite clauses, to tensed verbs, adjectives, and participles. 1.3.1 Matrix Predicate Agreement The finite verb agrees with the nominative subject in number and person. Adjectives and certain Participles agree with the subject in number, gender, and case. Other participles are simply invariable. 2

(5) a. Strákarnir höfdu verid duglegir. the-boys.nom.mpl had.3pl been energetic.nom.mpl The boys had been energetic. b. Stelpurnar höfdu verid duglegar. the-girls.nom.fpl had.3pl been energetic.nom.fpl The girls had been energetic. c. Börnin höfdu verid dugleg. the-children.nom.npl had.3pl been energetic.nom.npl The children had been energetic. If we put the above sentences in an ECM environment where the subject gets accusative case, the predicate agrees with the subject and appears in the accusative. Of course, ECM environments are non-finite so we cannot say anything about finite verb agreement there. But sometimes the agreement pattern is different. (6) a. Strákunum hafdi verid kalt. the-boys.dat had.default been cold.default The boys had been cold. b. Stelpunum hafdi verid kalt. the-girls.dat had.default been cold.default The girls had been cold. c. Okkur hafdi verid kalt. we.dat had.default been cold.default We had been cold. In general, Quirky Subjects never trigger agreement. Sometimes the agreement is with the object. (7) a. Okkur höfdu leidst strákarnir. we.dat had.3pl bored the-boys.nom.3pl We had been bored by the boys. b. Okkur höfdu verid sagdar sögurnar ádur. we.dat had.3pl been told.nom.fpl the-stories.nom.fpl before We had been told the stories before. And sometimes the agreement is with nothing. (8) Okkur hafdi verid sagt frá Pessu ádur. we.dat had.default been told.default about this.dat before We had been told about this before. 3

1.3.2 Agreement in Control Environments (9) a. Strákarnir voru adstodadir/*adstodad. the-boys.nom were aided.nom.mpl/aided.default The boys were aided. b. Strákarnir vonast til [ad The boys hope to be aided. PRO verda adstodadir/*adstodad]. be aided.nom.mpl/aided.default (10) a. Strákunum var hjálpad/*hjálpadir/*hjálpudum. the-boys.dat was helped.default/helped.nom.mpl/helped.dat.mpl The boys were helped. b. Strákarnir vonast til [ad PRO verda the-boys.dat hope for to be hjálpad/*hjálpadir/*hjálpudum]. helped.default/helped.nom.mpl/helped.dat.mpl The boys hoped to be helped by somebody. (11) In finite clauses, get to the party and be elected assign nominative to their subjects. a. want assigns Quirky Accusative to its subject: Strákana langadi til [ad the-boys.acc wanted.default for to The boys wanted to get to the party. b. bored assigns Quiry Dative to its subject: Strákunum leiddist [ad the-boys.dat bored.default to PRO komast allir get PRO verda kosnir be The boys were annoyed to be elected to the board. í veisluna]. all.nom.mpl to the-party í stjórnina]. elected.nom.mpl to the-board 1.4 A comparison with Hindi-Urdu Hindi-Urdu also allows for non-nominative subjects. (12) us-ko vo kitaab pasand nahĩ: aa-ii. s/he-dat that book.f.nom pleasing come-pfv.f S/he liked that book. But it differs in what can be controlled (i.e. realized as PRO). (13) a. mẽ [un logõ-ko PRO pasand aa-naa] nahĩ chaah-taa I.Nom those people-dat pleasing come-inf Neg want-impfv.msg I don t want those people to like me. b. *mẽ [PRO vo I.Nom that I don t want to like that book. kitaab pasand aa-naa] nahĩ chaah-taa book pleasing come-inf Neg want-impfv.msg 4

How do Hindi-Urdu and Icelandic differ with respect to control into non-nominative subject constructions? Assuming that PRO in Hindi-Urdu can be taken to have properties similar to that of PRO in English, articulate the difference between Icelandic and Hindi-Urdu in this domain in terms of the properties of their null infinitival subjects and other independent properties of the two languages. 2 Hindi-Urdu Again Provide an analysis of the agreement facts seen in the following data. Compare them with the Icelandic facts. The rest of the problem examines the intricacies of the passive in Hindi-Urdu. For concreteness assume that v AG, perfective aspect, and finite T 0 assign ergative case to subjects. You should also assume that the φ-features of any DP with an overt case-clitic (the Ergative -ne and the Dative/Accusative -ko) are invisible. Please feel free to email me if you need additional data. 2.1 Promotion It is unclear if there is ever promotion in Hindi-Urdu passives. This is because accusative and nominative are both unmarked, word order is not informative, and given the right configurations objects can trigger agreement. (14) Split Ergativity: a. Perfective transitive, Ergative Subject: Ram-ne yeh ṭehnii kal kaaṭ-ii thii. Ram-Erg this branch.f yesterday cut-pfv.f be.pst.f Ram had cut this branch yesterday. b. Non-Perfective transitive, Nominative Subject: (15) Passive: Ram yeh ṭehnii kal kaaṭ-egaa Ram.M this branch.f tomorrow cut-fut.3msg Ram will cut this branch tomorrow. yeh ṭehnii kal kaaṭ-ii gayii thii. this branch.f yesterday cut-pfv.f Pass.Pfv.f be.pst.f This branch was cut yesterday. Thus we do not know whether this branch in (15) has nominative case (i.e. case-licensed by T 0 ) or accusative (i.e. case-licensed by the specifier-less v AG ). 5

Non-pronominal/Non-Proper name NPs in Hindi-Urdu can optionally be marked with a marker -ko that contributes specificity. -ko can also appear on the unique argument of a passive. (16) -ko-marked objects: a. Perfective transitive, Ergative Subject: Ram-ne is ṭehnii-ko kal kaaṭ-aa thaa. Ram-Erg this.obl branch.f-acc yesterday cut-pfv.default be.pst.default Ram had cut this branch yesterday. b. Non-Perfective transitive, Nominative Subject: Ram is ṭehnii-ko kal kaaṭ-egaa Ram.M this.obl branch.f-acc tomorrow cut-fut.3msg Ram will cut this branch tomorrow. (17) Passive with -ko retention: is ṭehnii-ko kal kaaṭ-aa gayaa thaa this.obl branch-acc yesterday cut-pfv.default Pass.Pfv.Default be.pst.default The branch was cut yesterday. Since there are independent reasons to believe that -ko never appears on elements that are caselicensed by T 0 (e.g. subjects of transitives), we can safely assume that this branch in (17) is definitely accusative (case-licensed by the specifier-less v AG ). But then we still do not have a clear cut case where we can be certain that there is promotion (i.e. the unique argument of the passive is case-licensed by T 0 ). 2.2 Proper Names and Pronouns Now consider the following data. Do they provide evidence of obligatory promotion? Your answer should include a discussion of why you think the data tells us what you think it does. Unlike other direct objects, proper names and human pronominal direct objects must be -ko marked. This is part of a crosslinguistically attested phenomena known as Differential Object Marking (DOM). (18) a. Ram-ne mujhe/*mẽ baazaar-mẽ dekh-aa thaa Ram-Erg I.Dat/I market-in see-pfv.default be.pst.default Ram had seen me in the market. b. Ram-ne Rina-ko/*Rina baazaar-mẽ dekh-aa thaa Ram-Erg Rina.f-Dat/Rina.f market-in see-pfv.default be.pst.default Ram had seen Rina in the market. Now consider the passives: (19) with -ko: a. mujh-ko baazaar-mẽ dekh-aa gayaa thaa I.Obl-Acc market-in see-pfv.default Pass.Pfv.Default be.pst.default I had been seen in the market. 6

b. Rina-ko baazaar-mẽ dekh-aa gayaa thaa Rina-Acc market-in see-pfv.default Pass.Pfv.Default be.pst.default (20) without -ko: Rina had seen in the market. a. (assume speaker is a woman) mẽ I.f baazaar-mẽ dekh-ii gayii thii market-in see-pfv.f Pass.Pfv.F be.pst.f I had been seen in the market. b. Rina baazaar-mẽ dekh-ii gayii thii Rina.f market-in see-pfv.f Pass.Pfv.F be.pst.f Rina had been seen in the market. 2.3 Subject Case in Infinitivals Infinitival subjects in Hindi-Urdu are in general either null (i.e. PRO) or genitive marked (you do not need to provide an explanation as to how Genitive is assigned). This has been taken to suggest that infinitivals in Hindi-Urdu are really gerunds. (21) a. PRO: [PRO mehnat kar-naa] acchii baat hai hardwork do-inf good thing be.prs.default To work hard is a good thing. b. Genitive Subject: [Ram-kaa is Ram-Gen this tarah mehnat kar-naa] acchii baat hai way hardwork do-inf good thing be.prs.default For Ram to work hard this way is a good thing. The subjects of transitive predicates cannot appear in the nominative: (22) *[Ram is tarah mehnat kar-naa] acchii baat hai Ram.Nom this way hardwork do-inf good thing be.prs.default (The subject is simply unmarked, I am assuming it s in the nominative.) With passives, we find the following. (23) a. DPs that don t need -ko as regular direct objects: [peṛ-kaa/peṛ-ko/peṛ is tarah-se kaaṭ-aa jaa-naa] sharam-kii baat hai tree-gen/tree-acc/tree this.obl way-in cut-pfv Pass-Inf shame-gen.f thing.f is For the tree to be cut down like this is a matter of shame. b. DPs that need -ko as regular direct objects: [Rina-kaa/Rina-ko/*Rina baazaar-mẽ dekh-aa jaa-naa] sharam-kii baat hai Rina-Gen/Rina-Acc/Rina.f market-in see-pfv Pass-Inf shame-gen.f thing.f is For Rina to be seen in the market is a matter of shame. 7

Finally, with unaccusatives a third pattern is found: (24) a. DPs that don t need -ko as regular direct objects: [khatõ-kaa/khat/*khatõ-ko der-se aa-naa] aam baat hai letters-gen/letter(s)/letters-acc delay-with come-inf common thing is For letters to come later is common. b. DPs that need -ko as regular direct objects: What s going on? [Rina-kaa/*Rina/*Rina-ko der-se Rina-Gen/Rina/Rina-Acc delay-with Rina s coming late is a common occurrence. aa-naa] aam baat hai come-inf common thing is 8